Are Religious People Happier?

by jp1692 45 Replies latest members private

  • Aaron James Eldridge
    Aaron James Eldridge

    Are Religious People Happier?

    This is, has been, and will be an exciting topic in the scientific study of religion. I will post a few articles at the end.The short answer, which is almost good for nothing when you start deconstructing it is YES.

    As, Never A JW, pointed out that it might be happiness like someone who likes drugs is happier when they are on drugs.

    jp1692, you are correct to ask happier than what. The way psychologist frame it is in terms of psychological well-being and flourishing. These are both well defined and objective, rather than self-reported, measures of positive psychological functioning. The psychological well-being has been more intensively investigated and there some really solid measure of the construct, I will post an early article by Carol Ryftt.

    So, if there are objective measures out there, akin to the IQ test or big-five personality test, then we can ask the question, "Is there a higher level of psychological well-being in people who are religious than people who are not religious?". For that question the answer is resoundingly YES.

    BUT 1.How is religion defined? 2.How often does one have to attend religious services? 3.Does it matter if the person is motivated by social norms versus an internal drive? 4.Does the specific religion matter? 5.Does it matter how the specific religion is accepted within its larger cultural context? 6.Does 'spirituality' produce the same benifits? 7.How the fuuuuuuuck do we define spirituality? 8. Would membership in any tight social group produce the same benefits as religion? 9. In our modern context, does religion make people more prosocial, i.e. produce people that provide aid to society? Arrrrrrrggggg so many questions!

    1. Really cannot answer this. It is a culturally specific definition that is driven by an individuals interest in the topic. A religious person will give it one definition, all the fields in the scientific study of religion have their own pet definition, and so on. Personally, I think it is like defining pornography. It may be dificult to define, but you know it when you see it.

    2. Well, this one has a bit of clarity. About weekly.

    3. Yes. Intrinsic motivation wins the day. It can actually be bad for your health to be religious just out of social obligation. Duh....I know, but this is how it is framed in research.

    4. Unknown and understudied

    5. Yes, quite a bit. Like being a Mormon in Utah versus rural Mississippi. Or Sunni living in Saudi Arabia versus next door to Donald Trump.

    6. Jury still out. Heavily leaning in the no direction, but that is debatable.

    7. No fucking clue. Seriously.

    8. Same as 6...Jury still out. Heavily leaning in the no direction, but that is debatable.

    9. Resoundly YES. But wait a second. I feel another question creeping up. Does prosociality go away if the religious person is rendering aid to someone in the 'out-group'? Damnit. Ok. But at least it can be that in general, religion makes people more beneficial to society than if they were not involved in any religion.

  • Aaron James Eldridge
    Aaron James Eldridge

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2017). Religious communities and human flourishing. Current Directions in Psychological

    Science, in press. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417721526

    Religious Communities and Human Flourishing

    Tyler J. VanderWeele

    Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health

    677 Huntington Avenue, Boston MA 02115

    Abstract

    Participation in religious services is associated with numerous aspects of human flourishing

    including happiness and life satisfaction, mental and physical health, meaning and purpose,

    character and virtue, and close social relationships. Evidence for these effects of religious

    communities on flourishing now comes from rigorous longitudinal study designs with

    extensive confounding control. The associations with flourishing are much stronger for

    communal religious participation than for spiritual-religious identity or for private practices.

    While the social support is an important mechanism relating religion to health, this only

    explains a small portion of the associations. Numerous other mechanisms appear to be

    operative as well. It may be the confluence of the religious values and practices, reinforced by

    social ties and norms, that give religious communities their powerful effects on so many

    aspects of human flourishing.

    Introduction

    Studies over the past several decades have provided increasingly strong evidence for an effect

    of participation in religious communities on numerous aspects of human well-being (Koenig

    et al, 2012; Idler, 2014; VanderWeele, 2017). While many of the early studies were

    methodologically weak, there is now a large body of rigorous empirical studies with

    longitudinal data and good confounding control (VanderWeele et al., 2016a) that indicate that

    religious community is a major contributor to human flourishing.

    Religion and Physical and Mental Health

    Longitudinal studies indicate that attending religious services at least weekly is associated

    with 25-35% reduced mortality over ten to fifteen years. The effects may be larger for women

    than men, and for black individuals than white, but they seem to persist across gender, race,

    and across different religious groups as well (Strawbridge et al., 1997; Hummer et al., 1999;

    Musick et al., 2004; Chida et al., 2009; Koenig et al., 2012; Idler, 2014; Li et al., 2016a;

    VanderWeele, 2017). One study indicated that if regular service attendance were maintained

    over the life course the lower mortality rates would translate into approximately seven

    additional years of life (Hummer et al., 1999).

    Religious service attendance is also associated with numerous health behaviors over time

    including less frequent smoking initiation, greater smoking cessation, less alcohol abuse, and

    less illegal drug use; attendance is not, however, strongly protectively associated with all

    health behaviors as the associations with diet, exercise, and weight appear more mixed

    (Strawbridge et al., 1997; Koenig et al., 2012; Idler, 2014).

    Religious service attendance is also longitudinally associated with better mental health

    including approximately 20%-30% lower rates in the incidence of depression (Li et al.,

    2016b; Koenig et al., 2009; VanderWeele, 2017), and with 3-to-6-fold lower rates of suicide

    (Kleinman and Liu, 2014; VanderWeele et al, 2016b). While cross-sectional studies suggest a

    protective association with anxiety, this does not seem to hold up in longitudinal analyses

    (Koenig et al., 2012; Li et al., 2016a).

    Religion and Social Relationships

    There is also evidence that religious service attendance is associated with better social

    relationships. Numerous studies have examined associations between attendance and divorce

    (Koenig et al., 2012). While many of these are cross-sectional, the longitudinal designs

    suggest that those attending religious services at baseline are 30%-50% less likely to divorce

    in follow-up (Strawbridge et al., 1997; Wilcox and Wolfinger, 2016; Li et al., 2016c). There

    are also longitudinal studies that indicate religious service attendance is associated with an

    increased likelihood of subsequently making new friends, of marrying, of having nonreligious

    community membership, and of higher social support (Strawbridge et al., 1997; Lim

    and Putnam, 2010; Wilcox and Wolfinger, 2016; Li et al., 2016c).

    Religion and Life Satisfaction

    Numerous studies have also indicated an association between attending services and

    happiness and life satisfaction (Myers, 2008; Koenig et al., 2012); almost all of these are

    cross-sectional, but the existing longitudinal evidence, controlling for numerous social and

    demographic covariates and baseline life satisfaction, offers confirmation of this (Lim and

    Putnam, 2010).

    Religion and Meaning

    Other studies have examined meaning and purpose. The vast majority of these have suggested

    that service attendance is associated with a greater sense of meaning or purpose in life, but,

    once again, almost of these studies are cross-sectional (Koenig et al, 2012). However, there is

    also some evidence that service attendance is longitudinally associated with greater meaning

    in life, even after control for social and demographic covariates and baseline meaning in life

    (Krause and Hayward, 2012).

    Religion and Virtue

    With the relationship between religion and virtue, once again, many of the studies employ

    cross-sectional designs. However, there is longitudinal evidence that those who attend

    services are subsequently more generous in charitable giving, more likely to volunteer, and

    are more civically engaged (Putnam and Campbell, 2012). There is also evidence that

    religious service attendance is associated with lower rates of crime, and while most of this

    evidence again comes from cross-sectional studies, the evidence from longitudinal studies

    appears to confirm this as well (Johnson et al., 2001; Johnson, 2011). In the case of character

    and virtue, there is also some interesting evidence from experimental designs, not specifically

    concerning religious service attendance, but other aspects of religions. There have been a

    number of randomized priming experiments suggesting at least short term effects of religious

    prompts on pro-social behavior (Shariff, 2016). There is also some experimental evidence that

    encouragement for couples to pray together increases forgiveness, gratitude and, trust

    (Lambert et al., 2012).

    Evidence for Causality

    There is thus evidence that religious service attendance is longitudinally associated with

    happiness and life satisfaction, physical and mental health, meaning and purpose, character

    and virtue, and close social relationships. A question that naturally arises is whether these

    associations are causal.

    Many of the early studies on religion and health were methodologically weak and used crosssectional

    data. This is problematic because of the possibility of reverse causation – that only

    those who are healthy can attend services. The only way to attempt to rule this out is to use

    longitudinal data collected over time and to control for baseline health (VanderWeele et al.,

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2017).

    As noted above, there are now numerous longitudinal studies examining service

    attendance and mortality, depression, suicide, divorce, etc., with good confounding control,

    and in these studies the associations still persist. Nevertheless, these studies do make use of

    observational data, and it is always possible that unmeasured confounding may explain some

    of these associations. It is, however, possible to use sensitivity analysis (VanderWeele and

    Ding, 2017) to examine how strong such unmeasured confounding would have to be to

    explain away the associations. For example, Li et al. (2016a) reported that to explain away the

    estimate of 33% lower mortality in follow-up for those regularly attending services, an

    unmeasured confounder that was associated with both lower mortality and greater attendance

    by risk ratios of 2.35-fold each, above and beyond the measured confounders, could explain

    the association away, but weaker confounding could not. Such substantial confounding by

    unmeasured factors may be unlikely, given adjustment already made for an extensive set of

    measured confounders.

    The corresponding measures to explain away the 29% lower depression incidence for those

    regularly attending services would be an unmeasured confounder associated with service

    attendance and lower depression by risk ratios of 2.1-fold each. And the corresponding

    measures to explain away the 84% lower suicide risk for those regularly attending services

    (VanderWeele et al., 2016b) would be an unmeasured confounder associated with service

    attendance and lower suicide by risk ratios of 12-fold each. In this case, extremely strong

    unmeasured confounding would be required. With observational data, one can never be

    certain about causality, but the results of sensitivity analysis, after extensive control for

    measured covariates, suggest that the evidence that some of the association is causal is quite

    strong. Another form of evidence that some of the association between religion and health and

    well-being is causal is that there are a number of plausible mechanisms.

    Mechanisms

    Numerous mechanisms, or potential mediators, have been proposed for the associations

    between religious service attendance and health. Assessing mechanisms is more difficult and

    the research on this for service attendance is not as strong (VanderWeele, 2015).

    Nevertheless, for the relationship between attendance and mortality there is evidence that

    social support, lower smoking, greater optimism, and lower depression may all be important

    (Koenig et al., 2012; Li et al., 2016a). Greater meaning and purpose in life and greater selfcontrol

    have also been proposed as possible mechanisms (Koenig et al., 2012).

    The existing evidence also suggests that the mechanisms may vary across outcomes. For the

    effect of religious services on decreased depression, the mechanisms of social support,

    optimism, and meaning in life might all be important (Koenig et al., 2012). For greater life

    satisfaction, the social relationships that religious services provide seem to play an especially

    important role, perhaps accounting for nearly half of the effect (Lim and Putnam, 2010). For

    the dramatically lower suicide rates among those attending religious services, while social

    support, less alcohol, and less depression may account for some of the effect, these factors

    may not be as explanatory as might be thought and the moral belief that suicide is wrong,

    reinforced by religious communities, is perhaps here of considerable importance (Koenig,

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2017). With lower divorce rates among those attending religious

    services, the programs within religious communities that support families and marriages are

    likely important, as are perhaps the teachings on love and sacrifice, the prohibitions against

    infidelity and divorce, and greater levels of life satisfaction and lower depression within

    married life (Wilcox and Wolfinger, 2016; Li et al., 2017).

    Another important mechanism relating religious participation to health may be the use of

    religious coping. Most Americans use religion or spirituality to cope with illness or stress

    (Koenig et al., 2012), and there is evidence that this likewise leads to better mental and

    physical health outcomes, at least in clinical contexts (Pargament et al., 2004). Religious

    coping may help in finding meaning and strengthening relationships in the context of

    suffering and illness. Another mechanism by which religious participation may affect health is

    that of forgiveness, the replacing of ill-will towards an offender with good-will (Worthington,

    2013). Many religious groups promote some notion of forgiveness. The existing research

    suggests that forgiveness is itself associated with better mental health, and possibly with

    better physical health (Toussaint et al., 2015). Relatively strong evidence comes from

    randomized trials of interventions to promote forgiveness: meta-analyses indicate that these

    forgiveness interventions have beneficial effects not only on forgiveness, but also on

    depression, anxiety, and hope (Wade et al., 2014). Although most of these forgiveness

    interventions do require a trained professional, there is some preliminary randomized trial

    evidence that even workbook forgiveness interventions, that can be done on one’s own, are

    effective in bringing about forgiveness and perhaps alleviating depression (Harper et at.,

    2014; cf. http://www.evworthington-forgiveness.com/diy-workbooks). Religious groups

    promote forgiveness and forgiveness itself can restore relationships, and improve mental

    health and well-being. There thus appear to be many pathways from religious service

    attendance to health and well-being.

    Religious Community

    An interesting aspect of the religious participation research is that it suggests that it is

    religious service attendance, rather self-assessed spirituality or religiosity, or private practices,

    that most powerfully predicts health and well-being. Private practices, spiritual or religious

    identity, and religious coping are all more weakly associated with health (Musick et al., 2004;

    VanderWeele et al., 2017). Religious identity and private spiritual practices may of course

    still be important and meaningful within the context of religious life, but they do not appear to

    affect health and well-being as strongly. The communal element seems essential.

    This also raises the question as to whether it is just community that matters, and whether any

    community would be as effective. While social support is an important mechanism relating

    religious service attendance to better health and lower mortality it seems to only explain about

    a quarter of the effect (Li et al., 2016a). Moreover some data indicate that, religious service

    attendance is a stronger predictor of health and longevity than any other social support

    variable, including being married, number of close friends, number of close relatives, having

    recently seen a friend, or a relative, and hours spent in social groups (Li et al., 2016a).

    Certainly other measures of social support and community participation do seem to be

    associated with better health as well but the existing evidence suggests that the effects are not

    as strong, nor over such a broader range of outcomes; moreover weekly participation in

    religious services – still at 36% in the United States – seems to be a far more common form of

    community involvement than any other (VanderWeele, 2017).

    Nevertheless, given the diversity of the mechanisms, we might wonder how many of them

    really are fundamentally religious in nature. While many of the mechanisms relating religion

    to health – social support, smoking, meaning and purpose, optimism – are seemingly not

    distinctively religious, and could be operative in other contexts as well, some of these are

    arguably quite central to religious practice. Greater optimism and less depression may result

    from religious messages of faith and hope; meaning and purpose follow directly from

    profound religious understandings of the world and the place of human persons in it; even

    with something as seemingly mundane as less smoking, religious teachings that the body is a

    gift from God wherein the spirit dwells may have some effect on altering such behaviors.

    Thus, religious ideas may in fact be intertwined with many of these mechanisms. It is perhaps

    the bringing together of the religious and the social that gives religious service attendance its

    powerful effects.

    Negative Effects of Religious Community

    Of course religious service attendance and participation can potentially have detrimental

    effects as well. There is some evidence that the effect of attendance is less pronounced and

    even detrimental in countries which restrict freedoms (Hayward and Elliott, 2014); students in

    schools where their own religious affiliation is in the minority may be more likely to attempt

    suicide or self-harm (Young et al. 2011); in one study, religious participation was associated

    with higher depression rates for unwed mothers (Koenig, 2009). Spiritual struggles have also

    been shown to be associated longitudinally with worse health (Pargament et al., 2004), and

    negative congregational interactions are associated with lower measures of well-being

    (Ellison et al., 2009). While much of the evidence thus points to a beneficial effect of

    religious participation on health, it is clear that there are contexts and settings for which this is

    not so. Such research can also be of importance to religious communities in informing

    communal and pastoral practices.

    Human Flourishing, Society, and the Ends of Religion

    The review here has focused on religious community and individual flourishing. However,

    there is of course a broader societal dimension, which should be considered when assessing

    religion’s contribution to human well-being both generally and also towards those who do not

    or no longer participate in religious communities. We have not, for example, discussed

    religious acts of terrorism, or child sexual abuse in religious contexts. While abuse rates may

    be even higher in the general population (Koenig, 2017), the fact that they took place at all in

    religious contexts is very troubling. In evaluating the contribution of religious communities to

    flourishing, one would also want to take into account these problematic aspects as well, but

    similarly likewise the many contributions of religious communities to broader society as well

    such as food pantries, soup kitchens, prison outreach, counseling, civil rights, and Alcoholics

    Anonymous services (Idler, 2014; Levin, 2016), as well as the extensive provision of medical

    care. In some African countries faith-based organizations may provide as much as half of all

    care (Idler, 2014). These are all undoubtedly crucial in evaluating the role of religion in

    society.

    An even broader perspective might consider the historical contributions of religious

    communities, both positive and negative, such as the role such communities did or did not

    have in so-called wars of religion, and also in the development of hospitals, economics, law,

    human rights, science, and the preservation of learning (Carroll and Shiflett, 2001;

    Cavanaugh, 2009; Woods and Canizares, 2012).

    But a yet broader perspective still would also consider what religious communities view as

    their own ends and purposes. Of course, neither health, nor worldly satisfaction, is the

    primary focus of the world’s major religious traditions. Instead, a vision of or communion

    with God, or the living life as God intended, or a restoration to complete wholeness, are often

    central in the primary ends of religious communities (Aquinas, 1948; Catholic Church, 2000;

    Westminister, 2014; Koenig et al., 2012). Many religious communities teach that ultimate

    well-being extends beyond flourishing in this life and that these final ends of religion are to be

    given greater value. Given the focus of religion on the transcendent, it is thus perhaps

    remarkable that participation in religious communities affects so many human flourishing

    outcomes in life, here and now, as well.

    Acknowledgements

    This work was funded by the Templeton Foundation and by the Program on Integrative

    Knowledge and Human Flourishing at Harvard University.

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2017). Religious communities and human flourishing. Current Directions in Psychological

    Science, in press. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417721526

    References

    Aquinas, T. (1274/1948) Summa Theologica. Complete English Translation in Five Volumes,

    Notre Dame, IN: Ave Maria Press. First Part of the Second Part, Questions 1-5.

    Catholic Church. Catechism of the Catholic Church. 2nd ed. Libreria Editrice Vaticana, 2000.

    Carroll, V. and Shiflett, D. (2001). Christianity On Trial: Arguments Against Anti-Religious

    Bigotry. Encounter Books.

    Cavanaugh, W.T. (2009). The Myth of Religious Violence. Oxford University Press: New

    York.

    Chida Y, Steptoe A, Powell LH. (2009). Religiosity/spirituality and mortality. A systematic

    quantitative review. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 78(2):81–90.

    Ellison CG, Zhang W, Krause N, Marcum JP (2009). Does negative interaction in the church

    increase psychological distress? Longitudinal findings from the Presbyterian Panel Survey.

    Sociology of Religion 70 (4): 409–431.

    Harper Q, Worthington EL, Griffin BJ, Lavelock CR, Hook JN, Vrana SR, Greer CL (2014).

    Efficacy of a workbook to promote forgiveness: A randomized controlled trial with university

    students. Journal of Clinical Psychology 70:1158–1169.

    Hayward RD, Elliott M. (2014). Cross-national analysis of the influence of cultural norms and

    government restrictions on the relationship between religion and well-being. Review of

    Religious Research, 56:23–43.

    Hummer RA, Rogers RG, Nam CB, Ellison CG. (1999). Religious involvement and US adult

    mortality. Demography, 36(2):273–285.

    Idler EL, Ed. (2014). Religion as a Social Determinant of Public Health. New York: Oxford

    University Press.

    Johnson, B.R., Jang, S.J., Larson, D.B., and Li, S.D. (2001). Does adolescent religious

    commitment matter?: a reexamination of the effects of religiosity on delinquency. Journal of

    Research in Crime and Delinquency, 38: 22-44.

    Johnson, B.R. (2011). More God, Less Crime: Why Faith Matters and How it Could Matter

    More. Templeton Press: West Conshohocken, PA.

    Kleinman EM, Liu RT. (2014). Prospective prediction of suicide in a nationally representative

    sample: Religious service attendance as a protective factor. British Journal of Psychiatry,

    204:262–266.

    Koenig, H.G. (2009) Research on religion, spirituality and mental health: a review. Canadian

    Journal of Psychiatry, 54:283-291.

    Koenig H.G. (2017). Catholic Christianity and Mental Health: Beliefs, Research and

    Applications. CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform.

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2017). Religious communities and human flourishing. Current Directions in Psychological

    Science, in press. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417721526

    Koenig HG, King DE, Carson VB. (2012). Handbook of Religion and Health. 2nd ed. Oxford,

    New York: Oxford University Press.

    Krause N, Hayward RD. (2012). Religion, meaning in life, and change in physical functioning

    during late adulthood. Journal of Adult Development, 19: 158–169.

    Lambert, N.M., Fincham, F.D., Lavallee, D.C., and Brantley, C.W. (2012). Praying together

    and staying together: couple prayer and trust. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality.

    2012;4:1-9.

    Levin, J. (2016). Partnerships between the faith-based and medical sectors: Implications for

    preventive medicine and public health. Preventive Medicine Reports 4:344–350.

    Li S, Stamfer M, Williams DR, VanderWeele TJ. (2016a). Association between religious

    service attendance and mortality among women. JAMA Internal Medicine, 176(6):777–785.

    Li S, Okereke OI, Chang SC, Kawachi I, VanderWeele TJ (2016b). Religious service

    attendance and lower depression among women: A prospective cohort study. Annals of

    Behavioral Medicine. 50:876-884.

    Li, S. and Kubzansky, L.D., and VanderWeele, TJ. (2016). Religious service attendance,

    divorce, and remarriage among U.S. women. Social Science Research Network. Available at

    SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=2891385 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2891385

    Lim C, Putnam RD. (2010). Religion, social networks, and life satisfaction. American

    Sociological Review, 75:914–933.

    Myers, D.G. Religion and human flourishing. In: M. Eid and R.J. Larsen (Eds.). The Science

    of Subjective Well-being. New York: Guilford.

    Musick MA, House JS, Williams DR. (2004). Attendance at religious services and mortality

    in a national sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 45(2):198–213.

    Pargament KI, Koenig HG, Tarakeshwar N, Hahn J. (2004). Religious coping methods as

    predictors of psychological, physical and spiritual outcomes among medically ill elderly

    patients: A two-year longitudinal study. Journal of Health Psychology, 9:713–730.

    Putnam RD, Campbell DE. (2012). American Grace. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Shariff A.F., Willard, A.K., Andersen, T., and Norenzayan, A. (2016). Religious priming: A

    meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Review

    20(1):27-48.

    Strawbridge WJ, Cohen RD, Shema SJ, Kaplan GA. (1997). Frequent attendance at religious

    services and mortality over 28 years. American Journal of Public Health, 87(6):957–961.

    Toussaint, L., Worthington, E., Williams, D.R. (2015). Forgiveness and Health: Scientific

    Evidence and Theories Relating Forgiveness to Better Health. Springer.

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2015). Explanation in Causal Inference: Methods for Mediation and

    Interaction. New York: Oxford University Press.

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2017). Religious communities and human flourishing. Current Directions in Psychological

    Science, in press. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417721526

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2017). Religion and health: a synthesis. In: Peteet, J.R. and Balboni, M.J.

    (eds.). Spirituality and Religion within the Culture of Medicine: From Evidence to Practice.

    New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    VanderWeele, T.J. and Ding, P. (2017). Sensitivity analysis in observational research:

    introducing the E-value. Annals of Internal Medicine, in press.

    VanderWeele TJ, Jackson JW, Li S. (2016a). Causal inference and time-varying exposures: A

    case study of religion and mental health. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,

    51:1457-1466.

    VanderWeele TJ, Li S, Tsai A, Kawachi I. (2016b). Association between religious service

    attendance and lower suicide rates among US women. JAMA Psychiatry, 73:845-851.

    VanderWeele TJ, Yu J, Cozier YC, Wise L, Rosenberg L, Shields AE, Palmer JR. (2017).

    Religious service attendance, prayer, religious coping, and religious-spiritual identity as

    predictors of all-cause mortality in the Black Women’s Health Study. American Journal of

    Epidemiology, 185:515-522.

    Wade NG, Hoyt WT, Kidwell JE, Worthington EL. (2014). Efficacy of psychotherapeutic

    interventions to promote forgiveness: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

    Psychology, 82:154–170.

    Westminster (1647/2014). Westminster Shorter Catechism. SMK Books: Radford VA.

    Wilcox, W.B., and Wolfinger, N.H. (2016). Soul Mates: Religion, Sex, Love, and Marriage

    among African Americans and Latinos. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Woods T.E. and Canizares, A. (2012). How the Catholic Church Built Western Civilization.

    Regnery Publishing, Washington DC.

    Worthington EL. (2013). Forgiveness and Reconciliation. New York: Routledge.

    Young R, Sweeting H, Ellaway A. (2011). Do schools differ in suicide risk? The influence of

    school and neighbourhood on attempted suicide, suicidal ideation and self-harm among

    secondary school. BMC Public Health, 11:874.

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2017). Religious communities and human flourishing. Current Directions in Psychological

    Science, in press. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417721526

    Recommended Readings

    Idler EL, Ed. (2014). Religion as a Social Determinant of Public Health. New York: Oxford

    University Press.

    Koenig HG, King DE, Carson VB. (2012). Handbook of Religion and Health. 2nd ed. Oxford,

    New York: Oxford University Press.

    Lim C, Putnam RD. (2010). Religion, social networks, and life satisfaction. American

    Sociological Review, 75:914–933.

    Strawbridge WJ, Cohen RD, Shema SJ, Kaplan GA. (1997). Frequent attendance at religious

    services and mortality over 28 years. American Journal of Public Health, 87(6):957–961.

    VanderWeele, T.J. (2017). Religion and health: a synthesis. In: Peteet, J.R. and Balboni, M.J.

    (eds.). Spirituality and Religion within the Culture of Medicine: From Evidence to Practice.

    New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Aaron James Eldridge
    Aaron James Eldridge

    The Ryff and Keyes article would not copy correctly but here is a link The Structure of Psychological Well-Being

  • Old Navy
    Old Navy

    No. Religious people are not "happier." While they may experience brief episodes of "happiness" in connection with certain activities, it is fleeting.

    In today's World is anyone at all truly happy?

    Some are content and serene as they await the future time of real happiness. They are the fortunate few.

  • mentalclarity
    mentalclarity

    This is a really interesting discussion! I haven't had a chance to read your essay JP but I agree with what you wrote:

    "This is in fact my personal takeaway from researching this subject: religions can, and often do, provide us with things that are necessary for human happiness: community, a sense of belonging, purpose and meaning".

    I also agree that you don't necessarily need to be part of a religion to enjoy the byproduct of living your life feeling connected to others/acting according to your beliefs (or not going against your beliefs) and knowing that your actions are beneficial for others outside yourself.

    I think in many ways, this is why Activism has somewhat substituted organized religion to an extent. It provides a sense of belonging, a community of like-minded, purpose and meaning. I think as humans, part of our make-up (as emotional/physical/spiritual creatures) is a need for these things and whether its satisfied by religion/activism/some sort of spirituality, if we lack it we are not at peace and restless.

  • Diogenesister
    Diogenesister

    I'm interested in your question and your replies. I can see how this animates you! I can relate...nothing like a bit of philosophical enquiry ( as you mentioned it is a false dichotomy though).

    I think there is a strange pattern in this. I think people who are not religious but have really thought through their reasons for non belief, and it's part of who they are, rarther than a 'culturally' western non believer are just, if not more, happier than the religious.

    Conversly I think people who have a small amount of religion in their life are probably happier than those that are bogged down with religious dogmas.

    So these two groups are happiest and there's probably not much in it.

    (A Psychologist would say " but you've just described yourself and people you associate with "LOL [people always frame themselves as happiest])

Share this

Google+
Pinterest
Reddit