FBI arrests Seven Members of NAMBLA

by DevonMcBride 29 Replies latest watchtower child-abuse

  • MonkeyPrincess
    MonkeyPrincess

    This is so disturbing and the first i have heard about them. I cannot beleive that a
    group such as this exist, let alone operates growing in members. This is just
    disgusting and i am sickend by this. I told my husband that i am going to start
    a crusade to stopping them. I feel that if I didnt even know about them prior to reading
    it on this thread, i would have never known.. how many other mothers out there don't
    know about them? Everyone should know so we can protect our babies.

    MP

  • abbagail
    abbagail

    OK, that turned out to be a big mess above. Sorry about that. I'll try to delete it in 30 minutes if it let's me. I'm using Netscape 6 for Mac, and let it "validate" the HTML before posting, and it said it was OK, so I thought it would "look OK" in print. - Wrong! :-/

    It was very interesting info, to me anyway, about the propaganda pushed by these NAMBLA people and others like them, and how they fund studies to try to "present a good side" to men/boy "love" (sick!).

    But in that condition (above) it's miserable to read. Sorry folks. I should stick to plain text only, lol.
    /ag

    PS: to MP, you could hook up with other orgs. I bet Claire Reeves, founder of
    "Mothers Against Sexual Abuse," would know about all about NAMBLA.

  • abbagail
    abbagail

    Part 1 of 2...

    This is from an email I put together back on 2/20/04.

    Further below is a little info about a book (Misinformation Concerning Child Sexual Abuse and Adult Survivors), but the main thrust of this info is the section of the book which covers a study done in 1998 by three sickos named Rind, Tromovitch & Bauserman, of which... "A number of commentators suggested that the study is pedophile propaganda masquerading as science".... and on the flip-side, the study was... "Not surprisingly [the study was] immediately embraced by pedophile organizations. The North American Man/Boy Love Association (NAMBLA), a political and educational organization that advocates for the decriminalization of ?consensual? pedophilic relationships..."

    That's enough to make the hair stand up on the back of your neck, eh? The book refutes the conclusions of this study, thank God. Read the refutation of the study further below... and don't miss this interesting part: "To make this determination [of whether the report was biased toward pedophile advocacy], one must first clarify what it is that people supportive of pedophilia are advocating. : THE GOALS OF PEDOPHILE ADVOCATES: Brief History of the Modern Pedophile Movement"

    And definitely don't miss this section... it just shows you how evil and wicked these perverts are:
    ..."pedophiles sought to make their unpopular philosophy more palatable to the public by using four main techniques to justify and normalize their philosophy and practices (see Table 2)." - Be sure to read these 'four main techniques' the pedo's use to brainwash society!

    And I just learned something new... it is this: ... Paidika: The Journal of Paedophilia...
    Ugh! They have their own "journal," how nice.

    If you've never heard of NAMBLA before, you've heard of it now! Yes, it also really exists, ugh! ... http://www.nambla.org/ I remember reading an extensive report online within the past year or two, at World Net Daily

  • abbagail
    abbagail

    continued from Part 1 above...

    ---

    World Net Daily http://www.worldnetdaily.com/ that said it has always been the "agenda" of the homosexual "movement" to completely brainwash and take over America and its children... (no offense to any "innocents" of that persusion on the board.)
    /ag
    -----------


    http://www.leadershipcouncil.org/Research/Disinformation_book/disinformation_book.html

    Misinformation Concerning Child Sexual Abuse and Adult Survivors
    (Charles L. Whitfield, MD, FASAM; Joyanna Silberg, PhD; and Paul Jay Fink, MD, Eds.)
    Haworth Press, 2002 (Purchase book from Haworth)

    From the Foreword, by Robert Geffner, PhD, editor of the Journal of Child Sexual Abuse:

    ?Too often, the public and some professionals have been misled by media publicity and articles . . . that appear scientific, but in reality, are biased opinions or over-generalized research. Forensic cases are being decided in many courts based upon the recommendations of so-called ?expert witnesses? who do not actually know the clinical research or understand the dynamics of such abusive relationships.? This much-needed book points out and corrects misinformation that everyone who works with victims, offenders, or families in which sexual abuse has occurred needs to understand clearly. Especially vital in today?s political climate, Misinformation Concerning Child Sexual Abuse and Adult Survivors gives you state-of-the-science information on such myths as ?false memory syndrome,? ?recovered memory therapy,? and the ?lack of harm? to some sexually abused boys.

    CONTENTS:
    Introduction: Exposing Misinformation Concerning Child Sexual Abuse and Adult Survivors Misinformation/Disinformation About Child Sexual Abuse by Charles L. Whitfield, Joyanna Silberg, and Paul Jay Fink
    MISINFORMATION/DISINFORMATION ABOUT CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE
    Crisis or Creation? A Systematic Examination of ?False Memory Syndrome? by Stephanie J. Dallam
    The Legend of Robert Halsey by Ross E. Cheit
    FORENSIC ISSUES
    The ?False Memory? Defense: Using Disinformation and Junk Science In and Out of Court by Charles L. Whitfield
    (Mis)Representations of the Long-Term Effects of Childhood Sexual Abuse in the Courts by Daniel Brown
    LONG TERM SEQUALAE OF CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE AND CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
    Science or Propaganda? An Examination of Rind, Tromovitch, and Bauserman (1998) by Stephanie J. Dallam (reprinted below/ag)
    A Critical Appraisal of the 1998 Meta-Analytic Review of Child Sexual Abuse Outcomes Reported by Rind, Tromovitch, and Bauserman by John A. Whittenburg, Pamela Paradis Tice, Gail L. Baker, and Dorthy E. Lemmey
    The Real Controversy About Child Sexual Abuse Research: Contradictory Findings and Critical Issues Not Addressed by Rind, Tromovitch, and Bauserman in Their 1998 Outcomes Meta-Analysis by Pamela Paradis Tice, John A. Whittenburg, Gail L. Baker, and Dorthy E. Lemmey
    Implications of the Memory Controversy for Clinical Practice: An Overview of Treatment Recommendations and Guidelines by Christine A. Courtois

    HAWORTH PRESS
    10 ALICE STREET
    BINGHAMTON, NY
    13904-1580
    Tel: 800-HAWORTH, 607-722-5857
    Fax: 800-895-0582
    Email: customer@haworthpr essinc.com
    Website: http://www.haworthp ressinc.com/

  • abbagail
    abbagail

    Any mods that pass by, please feel free to delete the first entire "messed up HTML" post above. Sorry for the hassle! :-/
    /ag

    Part 3, continued...

    http://www.leadershipcouncil.org/Research/Disinformation_book/Rind_SorA/rind_sora.html

    Science or Propaganda? An examination of Rind, Tromovitch & Bauserman (1998)
    by Stephanie J. Dallam

    Reference: Dallam, S. J. (2002). Science or Propaganda? An examination of Rind, Tromovitch and Bauserman (1998). Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 9(3/4), 109-134.

    (Simultaneously published as a chapter in Misinformation Concerning Child Sexual Abuse and Adult Survivors (Charles L. Whitfield, MD, FASAM; Joyanna Silberg, PhD; and Paul Jay Fink, MD, Eds.) Haworth Press, 2002)

    Summary. An article, ?A Meta-analytic Examination of Assumed Properties of Child Sexual Abuse Using College Samples,? published in the July 1998 edition of the Psychological Bulletin resulted in an unprecedented amount of media attention and became the first scientific article to be formally denounced by the United States House of Representatives. The study?s authors analyzed the findings of 59 earlier studies on child * page 110 * sexual abuse (CSA) and concluded that mental health researchers have greatly overstated CSA?s harmful potential. They recommended that a willing encounter with positive reactions no longer be considered to be sexual abuse; instead, it would simply be labeled adult-child sex. The study?s conclusions and recommendations spawned a debate in both the popular and scholarly press. A number of commentators suggested that the study is pedophile propaganda masquerading as science. Others claimed that the authors are victims of a moralistic witch-hunt and that scientific freedom is being threatened. After a careful examination of the evidence, it is concluded that Rind et al. can best be described as an advocacy article that inappropriately uses science in an attempt to legitimize its findings.

    Article copies available for a fee from The Haworth Document Delivery Service: 1-800-HAWORTH. E-mail address [email protected] Webiste: http://www.HaworthPress.com ©2001 by The Haworth Press, Inc. All rights reserved.

    Keywords. American Psychological Association, child sexual abuse, ethics, pedophilia, sexual politics, scientific freedom, United States Congress.

    The Controversy

    A study entitled ?A Meta-analytic Examination of Assumed Properties of Child Sexual Abuse Using College Samples,? published in the July 1998 edition of the prestigious Psychological Bulletin, resulted in enormous social controversy and debate. The study?s authors, Rind, Tromovitch and Bauserman, analyzed 59 studies of college students and concluded that mental health researchers have greatly overstated the harmful potential of being abused. Despite finding that students who reported a history of child sexual abuse (CSA) were less well adjusted in 17 of the 18 types of psychological adjustment examined, Rind et al. (1998) suggested that the relationship may be spurious due to the confounding of CSA with family dysfunction. Rind et al. also reported that ?men reacted much less negatively than women? (p. 22) and that ?consent? was an important moderator of adjustment in males. They later summarized their findings, stating: ?We showed that for boys in nonclinical populations, willing relations are generally experienced positively or neutrally and are not associated with maladjustment? (Rind, Bauserman, & Tromovitch, 1999, p. 2185). Rind et al. (1998) went on to suggest that when labeling events that have ?heretofore been defined sociolegally as CSA,? scientists should focus on the young person?s perception of the experience: A willing encounter * page 111* with positive reactions would no longer be considered to be sexual abuse; instead, it would ?be labeled simply adult-child sex? (p. 46).

    Not surprisingly, the study was immediately embraced by pedophile organizations. The North American Man/Boy Love Association (NAMBLA), a political and educational organization that advocates for the decriminalization of ?consensual? pedophilic relationships, stated that the study confirmed that, ?the current war on boy-lovers has no basis in science.? NAMBLA also publicly thanked the American Psychological Association (APA) for ?having the courage? to publish the paper (Saunders, 1999).

    The study did not come to the general public?s attention until almost 9 months after its publication. Alerted by a listener, popular radio talk-show host Dr. Laura Schlessinger discussed the study?s findings on her show. On March 22, 1999, she told her 18 million listeners that she feared that the study ?could be used to normalize pedophilia, to change the legal system? (Duin, 1999). Soon after Schlessinger aired her concerns, a number of other public commentators severely criticized the study and the APA?s role in printing it. For example, in an article titled ?Lolita Nation,? newspaper columnist Debra Saunders (1999) stated that ?the APA showed appalling judgment in printing this pedophilia propaganda.?

    Political leaders were also disturbed by the study?s conclusions. Dr. Tom Coburn (R-Okla.) stated: As a practicing physician trained in science, I am shocked that the Psychological Association would publish a study that is clearly pedophilia propaganda masquerading as science.... The APA has brought itself and the entire psychological profession in disrepute by failing to filter junk science from a scientific journal. (Myers, 1999, p. 11)

    In Alaska, Rep. Fred Dyson introduced a resolution (HJR 36)1 calling on the APA to repudiate the study. The resolution was unanimously passed on April 30, 1999 and became a model for similar efforts in California, Delaware, Louisiana, Pennsylvania, Illinois, and the U.S. Congress.

    Negative publicity resulted in a number of press releases and statements by APA leaders. On March 23, 1999, APA released a statement that reaffirmed its strong historical stand against CSA and stated that, ?publication of the findings of a research project within an APA journal is in no way an endorsement of a finding by the Association? (American Psychological Association, 1999). However, on May 14, 1999, APA Chief Executive Officer Raymond D. Fowler, Ph.D., defended the study on national television (MSNBC), stating: ?It isn?t a bad study, it?s been peer-reviewed . . . it?s a good study.? On May 25, 1999, Fowler defended the study again in a letter emailed to APA division offi- * page 112* cers. Fowler said that the study passed a rigorous peer review process ?and has, since the controversy, been reviewed again by an expert in statistical analysis who affirmed that it meets current standards and that the methodology, which is widely used by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) to develop guidelines, is sound.? Suggesting that politicians and members of the media were misrepresenting the study?s findings to further their political agenda, Fowler assured the officers that the APA was, ?working hard to try to correct the record with those politicians and members of the media who care about the facts.?

    The study was also criticized by several scientific organizations. On May 24, 1999, the Leadership Council on Mental Health, Justice and the Media, whose mission includes ensuring that the ?the public receives accurate information about mental health issues,? issued a press release noting that Rind et al. improperly generalized from studies of predominantly noncontact experiences during adolescence in formulating some of their conclusions about the relative harmlessness of sex between adults and children. A few days later (May 27, 1999), Steven M. Mirin, MD, Medical Director of the American Psychiatric Association, expressed the Psychiatric Association?s disagreement ?with the implications of the authors? conclusions.? Mirin stated, ?From a psychological perspective, sex between adult and child is always abusive and exploitative because the adult always holds the power in the relationship.... Academic hair-splitting over whether the act should be considered adult-child sex or child sexual abuse . . . is not in the public interest and obfuscates the moral issues involved? (?Psychiatric Association Criticizes,? 1999). (well said!/ag)

    Apparently, the continued negative public reaction led Fowler to reconsider his support of the study. On June 9, 1999, Fowler hand-carried a letter to Majority Whip Tom DeLay (R-Tex) in which he admitted that the APA failed to ?evaluate the article based on its potential for misinforming the public policy process.? Fowler also acknowledged that ?some of the language in the article when examined from a public policy perspective is inflammatory? and includes opinions ?inconsistent? with APA?s policy on child protection issues. Fowler pledged that in the future his organization would be more cognizant of the potential for publications to misinform the public on important issues. Fowler also announced that for the first time in its 107-year history of publishing it has sought independent expert evaluation of the scientific quality of an article. The next day, Fowler (June 10, 1999) announced that the American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS) had been asked to do the review, ?because its credibility is unquestioned.?

    In July 1999, the meta-analysis by Rind et al. became the first scientific study to be formally denounced by the United States Congress. The House of Representatives and Senate both unanimously passed a resolution which re- * page 113* jected ?the conclusions of a recent article published in the Psychological Bulletin, a journal of the American Psychological Association, that suggests that sexual relationships between adults and children might be positive for children.? The resolution explained that ?elected officials have a duty to inform and counter actions they consider damaging to children, parents, families, and society? (House Con. Res. 107).2

    In response to criticism of their study, Rind, Tromovitch and Bauserman released a number of statements vigorously defending their results and conclusions. They claimed that their research ?brought methodological rigor into an area that needed this? (Rind, Tromovitch, & Bauserman., November 6, 1999), and suggested that they had ?an ethical duty? to report their findings (Rind, Tromovitch, & Bauserman, May 12, 1999). Claiming to be victims of political persecution, the authors characterized their critics as ?religious and moralistic zealots? (e.g., Rind et al., November 6, 1999). A flyer for a continuing education workshop about the controversy offered by Rind and Carol Tavris stated: The enemies of Galileo and Darwin, the enemies of the natural science model are alive and well.... Not only are the ?offending? data dismissed or trivialized, but the messengers can themselves be pressured into silence, recantation, or more simply be vilified by organs of academe and government alike. (?When Politics Clashes with Science,? 2000)3

    Concerned that the denouncement by Congress posed a threat to scientific freedom, a number of psychologists rushed to the study?s defense (e.g., Berry, 2000; Tavris, 1999; Woll, 1999). For example, Stanley Woll (July 26, 1999), Professor of Psychology at California State-Fullerton, suggested that Rind et al. were victims of a ?McCarthyesque witch hunt? which represented ?a dangerous assault on the process of scientific research in general.?

    The AAAS?s Committee of Scientific Freedom and Responsibility ultimately declined APA?s request for a review of the study saying they saw ?no reason to second-guess the process of peer review used by the APA journal in its decision to publish the article in question? (McCarty, 1999, p. 2). AAAS also reported that they ?saw no clear evidence of improper application of methodology or other questionable practices on the part of the article?s authors? (p. 3). However, they added that, ?if there were such problems, uncovering them would be the task of those reviewing it prior to publication or to readers of the published article? (p. 3). AAAS further noted, ?The fact that the Committee has chosen not to proceed with an evaluation of the article in the Psychological Bulletin should not be seen either as endorsement or criticism of it? (p. 3). Despite the disclaimer, Rind and Tromovitch viewed the AAAS?s * page 114* decision as a vindication of their work. Tromovitch stated: ?Their comments indicate to me that they consider our work to be up to par? (Burling, 1999).

    The APA indicated that it had no plans to ask any other organization to review the study. In early December 1999, Ray Fowler went on a protracted sick leave citing stress. The debate over the study?s merit remained unresolved. The purpose of the present article is to examine whether Rind et al. (1998) is best characterized as unpopular science or pedophile propaganda.

    THE SCIENTIFIC EVIDENCE

    When anybody makes a claim that is surprising or seems to be unlikely, science demands evidence. Thus, the first step in evaluating the conflicting views of the merit of Rind et al.?s (1998) work is to evaluate whether their findings are supported by appropriate data. Some have suggested that the results of the study by Rind et al. must be correct because the study passed peer review;4 however, it is important to note that passing peer review is no guarantee that a study?s results are correct. Without access to the original data, peer reviewers are often unable to determine the validity of a study?s results (Whitely, Rennie & Hafner, 1994). There are really only two ways to determine whether the results of a study are valid: (1) replication of the study?s findings using equal or higher quality methods, and (2) critical examination of the investigators? data and methodology.

    Comparisons with Studies Using Equal or Superior Methodology

    A review of the empirical literature examining the long-term consequences of CSA (child sexual abuse) call into question the validity of many of Rind et al.?s (1998) key findings and conclusions. For example, Rind et al.?s conclusion about the relative harmlessness of CSA conflicts with the findings of three previous meta-analyses of the relationship between CSA and maladjustment (e.g., Jumper, 1995; Neuman, Houskamp, Pollock, & Briere, 1996; Oddone & Genuis, 1996). In addition, little support can be found for Rind et al. conclusion that the significant relationship they found between CSA and maladjustment was likely spurious due to confounding between CSA and family environment. Table 1 summarizes the results of large scale representative studies, prospective studies, and co-twin studies using nonclinical samples. These studies, which are considered the gold standard in terms of validity and reliability, almost uniformly reported significant associations between reporting CSA and a wide variety of mental, physical, behavioral problems which persist even after controlling for family dysfunction.

    * page 115*

    Table 1. Results of Well-Designed Nonclinical Studies Which Controlled for Family Dysfunction

    Study Method Results

    Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor (1995)
    Random, nationally representative probability sample of 2,000 youths aged 10-16 years
    After controlling for family dysfunction, significant associations were found between CSA and increased levels of PTSD symptoms and school difficulties. Abused boys reported significantly more sadness then other children.

    Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor (1996)
    Longitudinal: Reinterviewed 1995 sample 15 months later
    Sexual abuse during the 15-month interim was associated with PTSD-related symptoms and depression not present prior to the assault.

    Dinwiddie et al. (2000)
    Co-twin: Examined twins discordant for CSA drawn from 5,995 Australian male and female twins.
    The twin reporting CSA consistently displayed more psychopathology then their nonabused co-twin. However, only a single outcome reached statistical significance - the association between CSA and suicidal ideation in males.

    Fergusson et al. (1996)
    Prospective study of a birth cohort of 1,019 male and female youths
    After controlling for family dysfunction, significant associations were found between CSA and higher rates of major depression, anxiety disorder, conduct disorder, substance use disorder, and suicidal behavior. Those whose abuse involved intercourse had the highest risk of disorder.

    Fleming et al. (1999)
    Subsample of 710 women selected from a larger study involving women randomly selected from electoral rolls in Australia
    After controlling for family dysfunction, significant associations were found between CSA and higher reports of domestic violence, rape, sexual problems, mental health problems, low self-esteem, and problems with intimate relationships. CSA involving intercourse was associated with the highest risk of disorder.

    Johnson et al. (1999)
    Prospective study of a representative community sample of 639 youths
    After controlling for family dysfunction, significant associations were found between CSA and increased rates of personality disorders during early adulthood.

    Kendler et al. (2000)
    Co-twin: Examined twins discordant for CSA drawn from a sample of 1,411 adult female twins
    The twin reporting CSA was consistently at higher risk for lifetime psychiatric and substance use disorders compared to their nonabused co-twin with odds ratios generally increasing with the severity of the abuse.

    Mullen et al. (1993)
    Stratified, random community sample of 1,376 adult women in New Zealand
    After controlling for family dysfunction, significant associations were found between CSA and higher levels of psychopathology, along with higher rates of substance abuse and suicidal behavior. A dose-response relationship (wonder what that means? drugs?) was found with those suffering the most severe forms of abuse having the greatest level of psychopathology.

    Stein et al. (1988)
    Random community sample of 3,132 male and female adults.
    After controlling for family dysfunction, significant associations were found between CSA and meeting diagnostic criteria for at least one lifetime psychiatric disorder, especially substance abuse disorders, major depression, phobia, panic disorder, and antisocial personality.

    Rind et al.?s (1998) finding that men react less negatively to CSA than women is another result that has not been supported by large scale studies of nonclinical populations. In this instance, Rind et al.?s conclusions are limited by the fact that studies of college students have almost exclusively examined internalizing behaviors such as depression, anxiety, or eating disorders.

    * page 116*

    Studies which include measures of externalizing behaviors have demonstrated that the aftermath for abused boys may be worse or more complex for boys than for abused girls. For instance, Chandy, Blum and Resnick (1996) studied over 3,000 high school students. They found that sexually abused male adolescents were at higher risk for poor school performance, delinquent activities, and sexual risk taking. Sexually abused female adolescents, on the other hand, showed higher risk for suicidal ideation and behavior as well as disordered eating. Similar findings were reported by Garnefski and Arends (1998) who studied a large representative community sample of adolescents. They reported that the experience of sexual abuse carried far more negative consequences for boys than for girls regarding the use of alcohol, aggressive/criminal behavior, use of drugs, and the amount of truancy, as well as regarding suicidal thoughts and behavior.

    Critical Reviews of Rind et al. (1998)

    Critical reviews of Rind et al. (1998) have raised serious concerns with the study?s design, statistical analyses, and conclusions. Some of the major criticisms are summarized below (also see Whittenburg et al. in this issue).

    Sample bias. A number of critics have noted that by restricting their analysis to convenience samples of college students, Rind et al. introduced a systematic bias in favor of their conclusion. Spiegel (2000) noted, ?By design, Rind et al. ignored those so mired in drug abuse, criminal activity, prostitution, or financial and educational hardship, that they could not get it to college? (p. 64). Dallam et al. (in press) raised similar concerns and cited research showing a strong relationship between CSA and academic difficulties or dropping out of high school. Dallam et al. also demonstrated that Rind et al.?s contention that ?the college data were completely consistent with data from national samples? (p. 22) was erroneous and was based on the misleading presentation of data from selected studies.

    Duncan (2000) tested Rind et al.?s (1998) contention that studies of CSA in college populations should be considered generalizable to the population as a whole. She compared the semester-by-semester enrollment of college students with and without histories of abuse and found that students with a history of CSA were more likely than their nonabused peers to display symptoms of post-traumatic stress and to prematurely drop out of college, especially after only attending one semester. Duncan concluded that it was likely that investigators using college samples see ?only the healthiest of survivors? (p. 987).

    * page 117*

    Measurement problems. Several critical reviews faulted Rind et al. for not standardizing their treatment of either their independent or dependent variables. For instance, Dallam et al. (in press) noted that Rind et al. uncritically combining data from studies of CSA with data from studies looking at other phenomena including consensual peer experiences, sexual experiences that occurred during adulthood, and homosexual approaches during adolescence.

    Holmes and Slap (1999) noted that Rind et al. uncritically combined psychological outcomes measured by different instruments with varying validity, relevance, and different interval scaling and cut points. After reviewing the Rind et al.?s study, Holmes and Slap concluded, ?meta-analysis is not appropriate when methodological rigor, let alone the question asked, is so varied? (p. 2186).

    Statistical analyses. Dallam et al. (in press) demonstrated numerous problems with Rind et al.?s statistical methods. For example, they noted that Rind et al. eliminated from analysis the studies of CSA which showed the highest degree of harm, while including studies that did not even purport to examine CSA. Dallam et al. also documented numerous instances in which Rind et al. misreported or miscoded the original data from the studies they analyzed. Moreover, these errors were consistently in the direction of CSA being portrayed as less harmful than the findings of the original study suggested.

    More importantly, Dallam et al. demonstrated that many of the findings that Rind et al. reported as being significant were actually statistical artifacts caused by their failure to correct for base rate differences in the rates of CSA in male and female samples. In this case, lower base rates of CSA in male samples caused effects sizes estimates for males to be attenuated and created the illusion that males were less harmed by CSA. After correcting for base-rate attenuation, Dallam et al. demonstrated that effect sizes for male and female samples were nearly identical.5 In other words, contrary to Rind et al.?s claims, males were not less affected by their abuse.

    Dallam et al. also found serious problems with Rind et al.?s moderator analysis of ?consent? by gender. Based on their analysis, Rind, Bauserman and Tromovitch (1999) reported that with abused boys ?willing relations . . . are not associated with maladjustment? (p. 2185). Dallam et al. reviewed the analysis and found that Rind et al. claimed to have measured a variable (i.e., willingness) that was not examined in the original studies. To get around this fact, Rind et al. assumed that detectable amounts of ?willing? sexual experiences were included in any study that did not explicitly state that the student should report only unwanted experiences. Dallam et al. examined the original studies and replicated the moderator analysis. They found no evidence to support Rind et al.?s assumption that the studies in the ?consent? group contained significant amounts of willing CSA. Moreover, after correcting for miscalculated effect * page 118* sizes and base rate attenuation, Dallam et al. found no significant effects for male gender and ?level of consent.?

    continued below

  • abbagail
    abbagail

    Part 4 continued...

    EVIDENCE FOR ADVOCACY

    Rind, Tromovitch and Bauserman?s attitudes toward sexual relationships between adults and children are important to evaluate for several reasons. First, the authors have claimed to be objective scientists who have suffered political persecution for publishing unpopular findings (e.g., Rind et al., November 6, 19999). Also, supports of the study have claimed that critics are trying to suppress their article?s important findings by unfairly tying the authors to pedophile groups (e.g., Tavris, 1999). To evaluate these charges, it is necessary to determine whether there is substance to the concerns raised by commentators who believe that their paper is better characterized as propaganda than science.

    Second, as the Rind et al. themselves noted, research findings can be skewed by an investigator?s personal biases. Rind et al. (1998) stated, ?Reviewers who are convinced that CSA is a major cause of adult psychopathology may fall prey to confirmation bias by noting and describing study findings indicating harmful effects but ignoring or paying less attention to findings indicating nonnegative outcomes? (p. 24). The fact that Rind et al.?s results differed from those of most other researchers who have studied CSA raises the possibility that Rind et al. may have exhibited a confirmatory bias in the opposite direction. (i.e., they ignored or downplayed findings indicating harmful effects).

    Finally, critical reviews of Rind et al.?s paper have invariably found evidence suggestive of bias. For example, after reviewing the study, Ondersma, Chaffin and Berliner (1999) stated, ?We believe that the primary flaw in the Rind et al. manuscript is not the science that it used, but its use of science. Through emphasis on certain key points and the omission of others, this article could be interpreted as using science to inappropriately question key moral and legal assumptions about CSA? (p. 3).

    Erikson (2000) found evidence of bias in the fact that when the meta-analysis indicated evidence of harm, Rind et al. immediately looked for an alternative explanation. Conversely, as Dallam et al. reported, when the findings went in the direction of the authors? personal beliefs (e.g., as in their analysis of ?consent?), Rind et al. ignored confounding variables and measurement problems. Moreover, as previously noted, Dallam et al. (in press) found most errors * page 119* in Rind et al.?s paper to be in the direction of portraying CSA being less harmful than the original studies? findings suggested.

    By itself, however, evidence for bias does not support the inferential leap that Rind et al.?s paper should be considered pedophile advocacy. To make this determination, one must first clarify what it is that people supportive of pedophilia are advocating.

    THE GOALS OF PEDOPHILE ADVOCATES

    Brief History of the Modern Pedophile Movement

    The origins of the modern pedophilia movement can be traced to the social changes that occurred during the 1960s and ?70s. Encouraged by the increased tolerance for minorities and alternative lifestyles, underground pedophile groups began to go public in an effort to garner greater social acceptance (Schuijer, 1990). However, the simultaneous rise of feminism led to greater public awareness about the negative consequences of sexual abuse. Recognizing the futility of seeking decriminalization of pedophilia at a time when abuse victims were speaking out, pedophile groups changed their focus. By the early 1970s, pedophile groups began to portray themselves as champions of children?s sexual emancipation (see Schuijer, 1990, p. 219). They conceded that sexual abuse was wrong but questioned whether all sexual contact between adults and children should be regarded as abusive. Claiming that children often initiated and benefited from sexual relationships with adults (BULL!) pedophile groups such as NAMBLA condemned the use of force and only advocated for the decriminalization of ?consensual? relationships.

    Although no formal ?agenda? has been published by organizations supporting pedophilia, the overall goals of pedophiles can be gleaned from a number of sources. Sociologist Mary DeYoung (1989), for example, reviewed the literature that pedophile organizations such as NAMBLA published for public dissemination. She found that pedophiles sought to make their unpopular philosophy more palatable to the public by using four main techniques to justify and normalize their philosophy and practices (see Table 2).

    Strategies for normalizing pedophilia have also been discussed in articles and books written by pedophile advocates and apologists. These strategies are summarized in Table 3 and briefly reviewed below. Because psychology is one of the primary sites for determining ?normalacy? in our culture, changing the views of mental health professionals has been recognized as an important predecessor of the lifting of legal restrictions on deviant sexual behaviors (e.g., Mirkin, 1999, p. 10).

    * page 120*

    Table 2. How Pedophile Groups Justify Having Sex with Children
    Techniques of Justification
    Description

    1 . Denial of Injury
    The use of anecdotal accounts of children who appear to enjoy sex with adults to demonstrate the benefits and advantages of such relationships to children. (SHOW ME ONE KID! THERE IS NO SUCH THING!/ag) Culpability for any harm that occurs to an abused child is displaced onto the reactions of others, such as the child?s parents, and the criminal justice and mental health systems.

    2 . Condemnation of the condemners
    Those who condemn sex between adults and child are portrayed as engaging in even more victimizing or exploitative acts then those for which pedophiles are accused.

    3 . Appeal to higher loyalties
    The assertion that they serve the interests of a higher principle: the liberation of children from the repressive bonds of society. Also, the attempt to align with other, less stigmatized, organizations such as the woman?s movement or the gay rights movement.

    4 . Denial of the victim
    The conceptual transformation of children from victims of adult sexual behavior into willing partners.

    Strategies of Normalization

    1. The adoption of value-neutral terminology. One of the fundamental steps that has been identified as necessary to change how society views pedophilia, is to change the language used to describe pedophilic relationships. Harris Mirkin (1999), Associate Professor of Political Science at the University of Missouri-Kansas City, suggested that before pedophiles can advance socially, ?neutral labels? must replace ?words like ?child molestation,? and ?child abuse?? (p. 12). A similar point was made by Gilbert Herdt, an anthropologist who has studied sex between adults and children in other cultures. Joseph Geraci, chief editor of Paidika, asked Herdt, ?Is there a social acceptance process that paedophiles can engage in and nurture to improve their situation? A normalization process?? Herdt responded, ?One of the steps would be the deconstruction of the language and categories being used. Buried in them are very, very old prejudices, fears and moral approbations? (Geraci, 1994, p. 17). According to Herdt, pedophile advocates need to replace ?dull and reductionistic? terms like pedophilia and abuse when discussing sex between ?a person who has not achieved adulthood and one who has? (p. 15). Moreover, words like ?child? or ?childhood? should be ?resisted at all costs?: . . . as soon as the category ?child? is invoked, everything is completely slanted and biased. It is suddenly no longer possible to have a rational * page 121* discourse. Indeed, you could say that the category ?child? is a rhetorical device for inflaming what is really an irrational set of attitudes. We would certainly have to use some other term. As I have said what we are discussing is desire, and the desire is for a person who is not yet an adult (p. 14).

    The importance of terminology was also discussed in the introduction to a special double issue of the Journal of Homosexuality devoted to ?Male Intergenerational Intimacy.?6 The special issue was guest edited by three members of the editorial board of Paidika: The Journal of Paedophilia. In their introduction, they noted that ?the choice of labels is never without political grounds or consequences? (Sandfort, Brongersma, & van Naerssen, 1990, p. 8). Because of its negative connotations, they avoided using the term pedophilia. In its place they used the terms man/boy love and intergenerational intimacy. They explained that they chose the term man/boy love as it stresses the ?unproblematic affectional side of the phenomenon,? and the term intergenerational intimacy was used because ?it gives man-boy contacts a less dangerous outlook? (pp. 7-8). (Wonder how they figure THAT?/ag)

    Table 3. Strategies for Normalizing Pedophilia

    1. The use of value-neutral terminology when describing ?intergenerational? sexual relationships. Also, avoiding terms that have negative connotations (including terms such as ?pedophile? or ?pedophilia?), and avoiding terms which call attention to the fact that the sexual ?partner? is a child.

    2. Doing away with the term child sexual abuse or restricting its use to behavior that is demonstrably harmful.

    3. Promoting the idea that children can consent to sex with adults. (THAT IS SUCH TOTAL BULL!/ag)

    4. Questioning the assumption that sex with adults is harmful to children. (It's NOT an ASSUMPTION/ag).

    a. Blaming harm on those who intervene after CSA is discovered or disclosed. (That is totally ridiculous! Kids WANT TO BE RESCUED!/ag)

    b. Publishing work showing positive or neutral effects. (No such thing as 'positive' or 'neutral' effects!)

    c. Proponents of man-boy love: Promoting the idea that studies showing harmful results are not applicable to boys; suggesting that boys are able to handle sex with adults at an earlier age than girls. (That is BULL also! Look around!/ag)

    5. Promoting the ?objective? study of sex between adults and children free of moral and ethical considerations. (They can try all they want, but they are fooling themselves/ag)

    2. Redefining the term ?child sexual abuse?. Another recurring theme among those seeking to normalize pedophilia is the need to redefine or restrict the usage of the term child sexual abuse. For example, Gerald Jones (1990), an Affiliated Scholar in the Institute for the Study of Women and Men in Society at the University of Southern California, suggested that ?intergenerational intimacy? should not be considered synonymous with child sexual abuse: ?The crucial difference has to do with mutuality and control? (p. 278). Jones suggested, ?Intergenerational attraction on the part of some adults could constitute a lifestyle ?orientation,? rather than a pathological maladjustment? (p. 288). However, before society could come to recognize the potential benefits of * page 122* intergenerational intimacy (p. 283 the use of the term child sexual abuse must first be limited to behavior meeting a strict definition of abuse (i.e., behavior that is demonstrably harmful).

    3. Promoting the idea that children can consent to sex with adults. The reconceptualization of children as willing sexual ?partners? along with the decriminalization of ?consensual? sexual relations is perhaps the key change sought by pedophile advocates. For example, on their website NAMBLA advocates for the decriminalization of ?consensual, loving? pedophilic relationships, but ?condemns sexual abuse and all forms of coercion? (http://www.nambla.de/welcome.htm). (The problem is that these pervs catch you so OFF GUARD you are STUNNED and it freaks you out and you just cannot speak. This occurred even as an adult when a perv was flashing himself all over the Mall! So much LESS as a kid! /ag)

    To counter arguments that children cannot give informed consent, some have suggested that with children consent should be understood as the simple willingness that an event should take place (e.g., Graupner, 1999, p. 31). Others suggest that a young child?s ability to form and vocalize preferences is sufficient evidence that they are capable of providing informed consent. For example, in his book Understanding Love Boys and Boylovers, David L. Riegel (2000) stated, ?Anyone who holds to the idea that a young boy cannot give or withhold informed consent has never taken such a boy shopping for new sneakers? (p. 38). (Hey buddy, sneakers are one thing, SEX IS ANOTHER! /ag)

    4. Questioning the assumption of harm. One of the greatest barriers to the decriminalization of sex between adults and children are the hundreds of studies demonstrating a consistent association between CSA and negative outcomes. Advocates of pedophilia have attempted to deal with this problem in a variety of ways. For example, they often any negative outcomes on parents or professionals who seek to prevent or intervene in the abuse. Riegel (2000), for instance, asserted: ?The acts themselves harm no one, the emotional and psychological harm comes from the ?after the fact? interference, counseling, therapy, etc., that attempt to artificially create a ?victim? and a ?perpetrator? where neither exists? (p. 21). Similar arguments are made by SafeHaven Foundation, an organization for ?responsible boylovers.? On their website, they wrote, ?The child abuse industry . . . takes a boy who has enjoyed pleasurable and completely consensual sexual experiences with another boy or man, and traumatizes him in an attempt to convince him that what he did was ?wrong.?? In addition, SafeHaven argues that, ?many of the supposed traumas elicited by psychotherapy turn out to be nothing more than the result of the False Memory Syndrome? (SafeHaven Foundation, 2001).7

    Another way that pedophile advocates seek to counter negative research findings is through the publication of positive ones. For example, Paidika advisor Theo Sandfort (1987) joined with members of the Dutch National Pedophile Workshops and studied sexual ?partners? referred by the organization?s members. The pedophiles helped fund the study and boys provided

    * page 123*

    glowing reports of the benefits they received from the relationships. More recently, a brief article titled ?A New Approach to the Societal Emancipation of Affectionate Relationships between Minors and Adults,? was published in the October 1997 IPCE online newsletter. The author, Titus Rivas (1997), stated that ?It is necessary to find adults who as a child or adolescent experienced a positive relationship with an adult, and who after having grown up, still maintain that they have not suffered because of that relationship on a long term basis, but rather keep cherishing the memory of it.? He solicits positive stories and cautions that it would be best if the referred adults were not now pedophiles. Rivas says that upon its publication, the report can be used it as ?a tool of emancipation.?

    One of the ways that advocates of ?man-boy love? deal with negative research findings is by claiming that they are not applicable to boys. Paidika board member Dr. Edward Brongersma8 (1990), for example, has suggested that research showing CSA to be harmful is unreliable because results involving female samples have been improperly generalized to males. According to Brongersma, ?Conclusions based on studying sex between men and girls should never be applied to sex between men and boys? (p. 152). Contending that boys are better equipped to handle sex at an earlier age, in his book Loving Boys: A Multidisciplinary Study of Sexual Relations Between Adult and Minor Males (Vol. 1), Brongersma (1986) asserted, ?A boy is mature for lust, for hedonistic sex, from his birth on; sex as an expression of love becomes a possibility from about five years of age? (p. 40).

    5. Promoting ?objective? research. Those who advocate normalizing pedophilia often argue that investigations of CSA have been distorted by our culture?s bias against sex between adults and children (NO DUH! /ag) (e.g., Brongersma, 1990). As such, they frequently call for a less emotional and more ?objective? and scientific approach to the subject (e.g., Geraci, 1994, p. 17; Jones, 1990). A study that is frequently cited as embodying the type of ?objective? research needed is Theo Sandfort?s (1987) research on boys? relationships with pedophiles. Although the Paidika advisor?s study is considered the epitome of objectivity by advocates of intergenerational sexual relationships (e.g., Brongersma, 1990, p. 168; Jones, 1990, p. 286), critics have pointed to strong strong evidence which suggests that the study was ?politically motivated to ?reform? legislation? (Mrazek, 1990, p. 318). Ethical concerns have also been expressed over the apparent lack of human subject safeguards and the fact that the boys were studied without the permission of their parents, many of whom were unaware of their child?s (often illegal) relationship with the pedophile (pp. 317-18).

    * page 124*

    THE WORK OF RIND, TROMOVITCH AND BAUSERMAN

    A review of the Rind, Tromovitch and Bauserman?s work reveals numerous intersections with pedophile advocacy organizations, along with frequent instances in which they have promoted the same changes advocated by those seeking to normalize pedophilia.

    Intersections with Advocacy Groups

    Both Bauserman and Rind have published in Paidika: The Journal of Pedophilia, a scholarly journal published in the Netherlands. The journal?s purpose was made explicit in its first issue: ?Through publication of scholarly studies, thoroughly documented and carefully reasoned, we intend to demonstrate that paedophilia has been, and remains, a legitimate and productive part of the totality of human experience. (?Paidika: Statement of Purpose,? pp. 2-3).9

    Bauserman?s (1989) article in Paidika reviewed the historical and cross cultural literature on sex between men and boys, and reported that ?man-boy? relationships have traditionally provided boys with positive male role models and teachers. Bauserman also contributed an article to the Journal of Homosexuality?s special double issue on ?Male Intergenerational Intimacy? that was edited by members of Paidika?s editorial board. Bauserman?s (1990) article consisted of a spirited defense of Sandfort?s research on boys? relationships with pedophiles. Rind?s (1995a) contribution to Paidika was a favorable book review of First Do No Harm: The Sexual Abuse Industry, a book which promotes the notion of ?false memories? being a major problem while at the same time arguing that sexual abuse is not necessarily harmful to children and that the age of sexual consent should be removed from the statute books (Goodyear-Smith, 1993). Rind (1995a) described Goodyear-Smith?s book as ?excellent? and argued that CSA is ?a social problem much less serious in size and scope than an unwitting public has been led to believe? (p. 83). (Blatant Liars, aren't they? /ag)

    After the publication of their meta-analysis in Psychological Bulletin, Rind, Tromovitch and Bauserman were the keynote speakers for an advocacy conference in the Netherlands. According to an announcement in the International Pedophile and Child Emancipation (IPCE) Newsletter,10 the conference was being convened ?expressly to throw light on the more positive side? of ?adult-nonadult sexual contacts? (?The Other Side of the Coin,? September 1998). The conference was hosted by the Foundation for Church Social Work in Paulus Kerk, Rotterdam, an organization headed by outspoken pedophile advocate Rev. Hans Visser.11 An overview of the conference appeared in an article in the local Rotterdam newspaper titled: ?Dominee Visser Pleit voor het

    * page 125*

    Aanvaarden van Pedofilie [Reverend Visser Pleads for the Acceptance of Pedophilia] (?Dominee Visser,? December 18, 1998). The conference also featured talks by two members of Paidika?s editorial board, Drs. Gert Hekma and Alex van Naerssen.

    In their keynote address, Rind, Bauserman and Tromovitch (December 18, 1998) claimed that their research demonstrated that little if any harm can be attributed to CSA and blamed the erroneous assumption that CSA causes intense and pervasive harm on child abuse ?hysteria?.12 After suggesting that the label of ?child sexual abuse? should be reserved for instances when ?early sexual episodes are unwanted and experienced negatively,? the authors concluded their presentation with the following disclaimer: ?We want to emphasize that our presentation should not be taken to advocate behaviors labeled [italics added] as CSA?. The full text of Rind et al.?s keynote address was reprinted in the International Pedophile and Child Emancipation Newsletter (Number E 4, January 1999) and is available online.13

    The Author?s Views and Positions

    A review of Rind, Tromovitch and Bauserman?s professional work shows that, at some time or another, the authors have recommended or enacted at least some portion of each of the actions outlined in Table 3. For example, Rind et al. (1998) recommended that professionals use value neutral terminology when discussing ?willing? sex between adults and children. Rind and Bauserman expressed even stronger views in prior writings. In 1993, they conducted research to determine how terminology used to describe the relationships between and adult male and a boy affected college students? perceptions of sex between adults and children. They concluded that the indiscriminate use of terms suggesting victimization and harm when describing ?adult-nonadult sexual relationships? can create a biased perception; ?researchers can give their readers the impression that . . . these relationships are abusive even when the evidence . . . points to neutral or even positive outcomes? (Rind & Bauserman, 1993, p. 266). In another paper, Rind (1995b) reviewed human sexuality textbooks? coverage of the effects of CSA, or what he termed ?psychological correlates of adult-nonadult sex? (p. 219). Rind objected to the use of terms such as victims, survivors, offenders, and perpetrators, as these terms confuse, ?harm done to children or adolescents with violations of social norms? (p. 219).

    In their 1998 paper, Rind et al. recommended restricting the usage of the term child sexual abuse to sexual episodes that are unwanted or experienced negatively. Such a recommendation appears to imply that Rind et al. believe that sex between adults and children can be noncoercive and that children can consent to sexual contact with adults.14 It should be remembered that this

    * page 126*

    reconceptualization was first proposed by Jones (1990) who suggested the change would help professionals recognize the ?possible benefits of intergenerational intimacy? (p. 276).15

    Bauserman and Rind (1997) argued that negative feelings are not inherent in sexual contacts between boys and adults, ?but depend on cultural views of these behaviors.?

    In the absence of social taboos and moral condemnation, negative feelings such as guilt and shame and doubts and conflicts about masculinity should not arise for children and adolescents who experience such contacts. The cross-cultural and historical literature provides examples of societies where sexual contacts between boys and adults, rather than being condemned and pathologized, instead were approved of, encouraged, or even regarded as necessary for healthy development? (Bauserman & Rind, 1997, p. 135).

    (Yeah, and no doubt it was the perverts of those days who wrote THOSE "cross-cultural and historical" reports as well. These heartless selfish apes need to go find their own planet and get the "H" outta ours. Grrr. /ag)

    --------

    FAIR USE NOTICE: This email may contain copyrighted material the use of which has not always been specifically authorized by the copyright owner. We are making such material available for the purposes of criticism, comment, news reporting, teaching, research, etc. We believe this constitutes a 'fair use' of any such copyrighted material as provided for in section 107 of the US Copyright Law. In accordance with Title 17 U.S.C. Section 107, the material in this email is distributed without profit to those who have expressed a prior interest in receiving the included information for research and educational purposes. For more information go to: http://www.law.cornell.edu/uscode/17/107.shtml. If you wish to use copyrighted material from this email for purposes of your own that go beyond 'fair use', you must obtain permission from the copyright owner.

  • stillconcerned
    stillconcerned

    does anybody know which one of the guys on the list is a minister?

    curious....

    kdn

  • RunningMan
    RunningMan
    Has anyone seen the Southpark episode on NAMBLA?

    What's so bad about looking like Marlon Brando?

    NAMBLA = North American Marlon Brando Look-Alike Association

  • I can be saved
    I can be saved

    I live 80 miles south of San Diego and this freakos were headed into Mexico and their "escapade" and were actually coming to my town. I woulda loved to seee them get caught here...give them the mexican jail pedofile treatment.... I am trying to get a hold of a friend of mine that lives in Canada. He was raised in the "good" faith and he is now going through serious mental problems after marrying into the faith AFTER he was defellowshipped and HAD 3 boys. Equivalent of suicide.... I am trying to get a hold of his parents through a kingdom hall in Vancouver, British Coumbia. Does anybody know of ANY # in the Greater Vancouver area to a KH? He is a good friend and I am worried for him, I lost my phone book with abouut 10 years worth of numbers and his was in there.

  • stillconcerned
    stillconcerned

    Nambla article.

Share this

Google+
Pinterest
Reddit